For the better part of the past 40 or 50 years, healthcare professionals of all stripes have been learning, certifying, and annually recertifying in that most fundamental of lifesaving skills, cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR). Countless hours have been devoted to palpating, looking-listening-feeling, ventilating, thumping, compressing, counting cadences, and attempting to coordinate all of the procedural components of CPR over and over again until we finally get it right—that is, execute the procedure as specified in the American Heart Association (AHA) guidelines. Every few years, when revised guidelines are issued, we struggle to break and unlearn the previously used behavioral sequences and embed new neuronal imprinting that coincides with the latest research evidence and best practices.

During more than 10 of those 40-plus years, that pristine moment of “getting it right” required a CPR instructor to observe our efforts and subjectively determine whether they collectively qualified us to be certified (or recertified) in CPR. We weren’t required to repeat a perfect behavioral sequence numerous times; a single correct demonstration could suffice (though that feat may have required hours to achieve). Some years later, CPR performance expectations were made more objective and quantitative when they were explicitly detailed with timing elements on a printed checklist. As CPR instructional technology continued to advance, we encountered mannequins with increasingly more sophisticated recording devices designed to objectively measure performance parameters related to timing, volume, and pressure and print out a strip for documentation. In addition to a passing score on the customary written test, CPR skill certification now required explicit, detailed, quantitative, objective evidence (ie, “a perfect strip”) rather than a generic, summary, qualitative, subjective judgment. Now, these mannequins are remotely controlled for a range of clinical data and clinical response and can record discrete and detailed measurements of whether performance meets the litmus test accorded by the AHA guidelines.

So what do we have to show for all of this invested effort? Keep in mind that the number of CPR certifications and recertifications need to be multiplied for each healthcare worker on an annual basis and then multiplied over 40 to 50 years. Do we see finely honed and flawlessly executed CPR skills, refined over decades of instruction? Can we point with pride to steadily rising CPR survival rates gleaned from these exquisitely sharpened resuscitative skills?

A substantial body of research and related literature1 27 attests to the fact that basic CPR skills decay at a rapid rate following CPR instruction. Within weeks of training classes, repeated measurements of CPR performance in simulated settings demonstrate that substantial numbers of healthcare staff no longer meet established standards. These findings hold true for all categories and levels of medical, nursing, and other healthcare personnel, as well as for lay persons, throughout the United States and internationally, and have held true for nearly 30 years, that is, since CPR performance has been examined.

Until quite recently, little was known regarding the quality of CPR actually provided to real victims of cardiopulmonary arrest. Two studies published earlier this year, however, offer evidence that deficient CPR performance is not only endemic in the classroom, but is generously distributed both within and outside hospitals here in the United States and abroad.

The 2 studies were roughly parallel in design. Wik et al28 measured the quality of CPR performed for cardiac arrest victims in prehospital settings by paramedics and nurse anesthetists in 3 European cities (Stockholm, London, Akershus [Norway]). Abella and colleagues29 measured the quality of CPR performed in a hospital setting (University of Chicago Hospitals) by that facility’s cardiac arrest response team. Both studies employed the same instrument to measure CPR quality: a prototype monitor and defibrillator designed by Laerdal Medical (Stavanger, Norway) that was capable of sensing the rate and depth of chest compressions, the rate of ventilations, and the fraction of time during the arrest that chest compressions were not provided (“no-flow fraction”). The 2 studies also shared the same outcome measure: adherence to established CPR guidelines.

A summary of the results from the Chicago hospital-based CPR study are provided quantitatively in the TableT1. A synopsis of these results can also be provided qualitatively, as follows:

  • Chest compression rate: too slow to be effective in many cases

  • Chest compression depth: too shallow to be effective in many cases

  • Ventilation rate: too fast for optimal ventilation in most cases

  • No-flow fraction: too long a period without chest compressions

Results from the European prehospital CPR study were even more deficient whether viewed with quantitative (TableT1) or qualitative lenses:

  • Chest compressions were not provided at all during 48% of cardiac arrest time. During the remaining 52% of the arrest time, chest compressions were provided at a rate faster than recommended. Combined (to account for 100% of the arrest time), the average rate of chest compressions would be 64/min, considerably slower than the recommended rate.

  • Chest compression depth: too shallow to be effective

  • Ventilation rate: slower than optimal

  • No-flow fraction: too long a period without chest compressions

The conclusions gleaned from combining the findings of these 2 studies are disappointing at best and deadly at worst. I suppose that the silver lining is that they help explain the persistently dismal (10%) survival rate after CPR that hasn’t budged significantly since CPR was first widely instituted. The study authors acknowledged this point and suggested that CPR quality might be enhanced by a greater focus on areas of deficiency during training courses1 and through development of mechanical devices that could deliver the desired rate and depth of chest compressions and improved patient monitoring with real-time feedback to rescuers when corrections to any of these parameters is warranted.2 

It is tempting to ascribe many CPR performance pitfalls to its emergency circumstances, its pressure-charged, often confusing, disorderly, typically unanticipated nature as well as its demand for therapeutic coordination amidst circumstantial chaos. Despite all these realities, there remains something especially elusive about performing CPR skills so that they conform to established standards. Although we often seem to seek answers to problems such as these by generating more and more detailed quantitative and technological “data,” somehow I’m not convinced that even more practice, more complex technology, more measurements, and/or more data will effect better CPR. My instincts as a BCLS, ACLS, and regional ACLS instructor trainer for many years suggest that the opposite may be true.

What is it about this set of behaviors that so often defies attainment? If other lifesaving emergency measures can be learned, executed, and performed “the right way,” such as extinguishing fires, the Heimlich maneuver, intubation, or defibrillation, why is CPR so often performed incorrectly? If practice makes perfect, shouldn’t we expect that 40 or 50 years of practice should suffice to perfect CPR prowess? Perhaps it is an opportune time to reconsider how we learn and retain such activities.

When we learn a group of related behaviors that are components of a more inclusive task or activity, we depend on a form of long-term memory called procedural memory, which enables us to gradually learn how to do things by repeatedly performing them. After sufficient repetition, we no longer need to recall the discrete sequential steps involved with performing that activity; we are able to execute the entire procedure without thinking about it at all. After we have acquired a procedural memory, 2 other effects become apparent: the first is an extended (sometimes lifetime) retention of how to perform that procedure. Thus, even if it has been years since we have ridden a bicycle or skied a mogul, once those activities have been thoroughly learned, we can quickly jump on a bicycle and pedal away or lock on our skis and head down a slope. In so doing, our so-called muscle memory enables us to quickly recognize and make any corrections necessary until our performance “feels right” to us. The second effect is that once memory of the procedure is formed, it becomes very difficult and disruptive to go back and refocus on the serial behaviors that comprise the activity we are accustomed to performing automatically. For example, any parent who has taught a child how to drive a car with a manual transmission knows how awkward and slow that teaching process can be when we haven’t concentrated on those serial details for 18 or 20 years. We repeat the phrase, “Wait, let me think about how to do this,” innumerable times because we struggle to return to thinking in those separate, sequential terms.

Once we have mastered the “correct performance” of the maneuvers that comprise CPR, does the persistent need for rescuers to focus on moment-to-moment details (eg, rates/minute and mm/compression, seconds without compression) not represent the same type of regressive serial thinking that would make us fall off a bicycle, wipe out on a ski slope, or stall a car with manual transmission? Once we have learned how to perform CPR (ie, have developed procedural memory for that activity), isn’t it just as unnatural and contrary to procedural learning to focus on those details? It may be that the current form of CPR performance prevents the formation of procedural memory or that the necessity of focusing on its constituent behaviors unravels whatever procedural memory existed.

Rather than searching for greater volumes or details in CPR performance data, perhaps a search for simplicity in these procedures would be a more promising approach. Consider the automated external defibrillator (AED). Not so long ago, only physicians could perform the complex and potentially dangerous defibrillation procedure. Today, with the help of user-friendly, ultra-simplified AED technology, anyone who can hear, read, or just follow pictorial directions can successfully defibrillate a victim of cardiac arrest. Perhaps there is a lesson there for CPR procedures. This month, the 2005 International Consensus on CPR and ECC [Emergency Cardiovascular Care] Science with Treatment Recommendations Conference publishes its latest guidelines. Let’s hope that they offer CPR guidelines that can actually be learned, retained, and provided to victims of cardiac arrest.

The statements and opinions contained in this editorial are solely those of the editor. To purchase electronic or print reprints, contact The InnoVision Group, 101 Columbia, Aliso Viejo, CA 92656. Phone, (800) 809-2273 or (949) 362-2050 (ext 532); fax, (949) 362-2049; e-mail, [email protected].

1
Assar D, Chamberlain D, Colqohoun M, et al. A rationale for staged teaching of basic life support.
Resuscitation
.
1998
;
39
:
143
–147.
2
Berden HJ, Willems FF, Hendrick JM, Pijls NH, Knape JT. How frequently should basic cardiopulmonary resuscitation training be repeated to maintain adequate skills?
Br Med J
.
1993
;
306
:
1576
–1577.
3
Boyde M, Wotton K. A review of nurses’ performance of cardiopulmonary resuscitation at cardiac arrests.
J Nurses Staff Dev
.
2001
;
17
:
248
–255.
4
Broomfield R. A quasi-experimental research to investigate the retention of basic cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills and knowledge by qualified nurses following a course in professional development.
J Adv Nurs
.
1996
;
23
:
1016
–1023.
5
Cooper S, Libby J. A review of educational issues in resuscitation training.
J Clin Nurs
.
1997
;
6
(1):
5
–10.
6
Curry L, Gass D. Effects of training in cardiopulmonary resuscitation on competence and outcome.
Can Med J
.
1987
;
137
:
491
–506.
7
Deliere HM, Schneider LE. A study of cardiopulmonary resuscitation technical skill retention among trained EMTs.
EMT J
.
1980
;
4
(2):
57
–60.
8
Donnelly P, Assar D, Lester C. A comparison of manikin CPR performance by lay persons training in three variatins of basic life support guidelines.
Resuscitation
.
2000
;
45
:
195
–199.
9
Fabius DB, Grisson EL, Fuentes A. Recertification in cardiopulmonary resuscitation: a comparison of two teaching methods.
J Nurs Staff Dev
.
1994
;
10
:
262
–268.
10
Freisen L, Stotts N. Retention of basic cardiac life support content. The effect of two teaching merthosds.
J Nurs Educ
.
1984
;
23
:
184
–191.
11
Gass D, Curry L. Physicians’ and nurses’ retention of knowledge and skill after training in cardiopulmonary resuscitation.
Can Med Assoc J
.
1983
;
128
:
550
–551.
12
Kaczorowski J, Levitt C, Hammand M, et al. Retention of neonatal resuscitation skills and knowledge: a randomized controlled trial.
Fam Med
.
1998
;
30
:
705
–711.
13
Kaye W, Mancini ME. Retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills by physicians, registered nurses, and the general public.
Crit Care Med
.
1986
;
14
:
620
–622.
14
Lewis FH, Kee CC, Minick MP. Revisiting CPR knowledge and skills among registered nurses.
J Contin Educ Nurs
.
1993
;
24
:
174
–179.
15
Mancini ME, Kaye W. The effect of time since training on house officers’ retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills.
Am J Emerg Med
.
1985
;
3
:
31
–32.
16
Moser DK, Coleman S. Recommendations for improving cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills retention.
Heart Lung
.
1992
;
21
:
372
–380.
17
Moser DK, Dracup K, Guzy PM, Taylor SE, Breu C. Cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills retention in family members of cardiac patients.
Am J Emerg Med
.
1990
;
8
:
498
–503.
18
Plank C. Effect of two teaching methods on CPR retention.
J Nurs Staff Dev
.
1989
;
5
:
145
–147.
19
Sternbach GL, Kiskaddon RT, Fossel M, Eliastam M. The retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills.
J Emerg Med
.
1984
;
2
:
33
–36.
20
Su E, Schmidt TA, Mann NC, Zechnich AD. A randomized controlled trial to assess decay in acquired knowledge among paramedics completing a pediatric resuscitation course.
Acad Emerg Med
.
2000
;
7
:
779
–786.
21
Ward P, Johnson LA, Mulligan NW, Ward MC, Jones DL. Improving cardiopulmonary resuscitatin skills retention: effect of two checklists designed to prompt correct performance.
Resuscitation
.
1997
;
34
:
221
–225.
22
Watson LA. Cardiopulmonary resuscitation competencies of nurses.
Int J Nurs Stud
.
1982
;
19
:
99
–107.
23
Weaver FJ, Ramirez AG, Dorfman SB, Raizner AE. Trainees’ retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation: how quickly they forget.
JAMA
.
1979
;
241
:
901
–903.
24
Wik L, Myklebust H, Auestad BH, Steen PA. Retention of basic life support skills 6 months after training with an automated voice advisory manikin system without instructor involvement.
Resuscitation
.
2002
;
52
:
273
–279.
25
Wright S, Norton C, Kesten K. Retention of infant CPR instructiion by parents.
Pediatr Nurs
.
1989
;
15
:
37
–44.
26
Wynne G, Marteau T, Johnston M, Whiteley C, Evans T. Inability of trained nurses to perform basic life support.
Br Med J
.
1987
;
294
:
1198
–1199.
27
Yakel ME. Retention of cardiopulmonary resuscitation skills among nursing personnel: what makes the difference?
Heart Lung
.
1989
;
18
:
520
–525.
28
Wik L, Kramer-Johansen J, Myklebust H, et al. Quality of cardiopulmonary resuscitation during out-of-hospital cardiac arrest.
JAMA
.
2005
;
293
:
299
–304.
29
Abella BS, Alvarado JP, Myklebust H, et al. Quality of cardiopulmonary resuscitation during in-hospital cardiac arrest.
JAMA
.
2005
;
293
:
305
–310.